For people with synthesthesia, sound has color and music has shape
By Lisa Marshall, Camera Staff
Writer
December 14,
2003
To Massachusetts photographer Marcia Smilack, the image
of light reflecting on water makes an almost deafening musical crescendo.
To New York painter Carol Steen, pain appears orange.
To Boulder poet Clara Burns, a year is an exaggerated oval, a "bent race
track," and each month is a colored rectangle within it.
Confused?
While these may sound like mere metaphors hatched in the fertile imaginations
of artists, they are in fact true sensory experiences, says psychologist Peter
Grossenbacher, head of the Consciousness Laboratory at Naropa University and one
of the nation's leading experts in synesthesia.
The little-known neurological trait — only recently acknowledged as real in
the scientific community — causes as many as 1 in 300 people to have their
perceptual wires crossed, leading them to respond to outside stimuli with one
sense instead of or in addition to the expected one. Some so-called
"synesthetes" literally see sounds and hear colors. Others taste shapes and feel
images. Many, like Burns, perceive time as having shape and numbers and letters
as having their own specific colors.
"For them, the experience is so real. A scent may be smelled and heard. It
has its own loudness, pitch and timbre. Or the sound of an electric guitar may
induce one specific color," says Grossenbacher, who has a Ph.D. and spent five
years studying synesthesia at the National Institutes of Mental Health before
coming to Boulder three years ago.
While reports of "colored hearing" and "colored letters" date back to the
early 1800s, synesthesia was long written off as the product of artistic fancy,
psychedelic drugs or madness. But in the past decade, thanks to brain scan
technology that now allows scientists to spy on the brain as it experiences the
senses, researchers have begun to acknowledge that the condition may be real and
investigate what causes it.
Perhaps, some researchers say, the brains of synesthetes are structurally
different, with portions of the brain that control color signals and hearing,
for instance, so close together that signals intended for one area hit both. Or
perhaps, as Grossenbacher thinks, people have the same basic neurological
wiring, but in most of us, hormones keep us from experiencing those extra
sensory perceptions so we don't get confused.
"There are people who never experience synesthesia until they take LSD," he
notes. Because people don't suddenly grow new anatomical connections in their
brains when they take drugs, he's convinced we all have the same neurological
connections, but for some reason synesthetes use theirs more fully.
The trait is worth studying, researchers say, because it may offer insight
into why some people are naturally more artistically inclined or more easily
overstimulated than others. And perhaps, some theorize, those who lose one sense
due to a brain injury may be able to compensate through synesthesia.
For those who have it and never knew it had a name, the brewing interest has
been a blessing because it has brought them together.
Being different
"People with synesthesia just think that everyone is perceiving what they are
perceiving until one day, they say something by accident and they realize that
is not the case," says Pat Duffy, co-founder of the American Synesthesia
Association and author of "Blue Cats and Chartreuse Kittens; How Synesthetes
Color Their Worlds," (Henry Holt; 2001) "It can make you feel kind of strange
and alone."
To Duffy, the letter A has always been orange in her mind's eye and the
letter C has always been blue. Time has always had shape and color.
It never dawned on her that others didn't see things the same until age 16,
when she was reminiscing with her dad about when she was learning to write the
letters P and R.
"I said to my father, 'I was so surprised that I could turn a yellow letter
into an orange letter just by adding a line," she recalls.
Understandably, he was puzzled.
Years later, she came across an article written by one of Grossenbacher's
colleagues, and learned that her visions of colored numbers had a name.
Soon, she was seeking out other synesthetes around the country so she could
share their story in a book.
Steen was one of them.
The New York painter and sculptor was 7 when she first realized she may be
different.
"I was walking home from elementary school in Detroit and I turned to my
girlfriend and said, 'You know, the letter A is the prettiest pink I've ever
seen," recalls Steen. "She gave me one of those withering looks and said, 'You
are really weird.' I didn't say another word about it until I was 20. I didn't
want to be weird."
In 1995, she helped Duffy found the American Synesthesia Association.
It now has 150 members and has held several national conferences at
prestigious universities.
Steen, known for her vivid paintings of the color of touch, now lectures and
writes academic papers about her experiences as a synesthetic artist. The ASA
Web site serves to bring synesthetes from across the country together.
"It has completely shaped my life, now that I know I have it," Steen says.
Artistic link
So far, research has shown that synesthesia is hereditary, and more common
among women and children. But new research is suggesting another
not-so-surprising trend. It is far more prevalent among artists.
Out of 84 synesthetes Grossenbacher interviewed this fall for a study, 26
turned out to be professional artists, writers or musicians and 44 spent much of
their free time pursuing the arts.
Another study, cited in an April article in Scientific American found that
the condition was seven times more common among artists than in the general
public:
"Our insights into the neurological basis of synesthesia could help explain
some of the creativity of painters, poets and novelists," wrote Vilayanur S.
Ramachandran, director of the Center for Brain and Cognition at the University
of California at San Diego.
In her book, Duffy points out that many famous artists over the centuries
have touted themselves as synesthetic:
Nineteenth-century French poet Arthur Rimbaud wrote about seeing colored
vowels; Russian writer Vladimir Nabokov described his own colored alphabet in
his autobiography; composers Frank Liszt and Olivier Messiaen both reported
seeing colored musical notes. Painter David Hockney paints with the colors he
sees when listening to music.
"It makes sense," says Duffy. "You have this very active inner life, and you
want to express it."
Massachusetts photographer Marcia Smilack says she could not practice her
unique art without her synesthesia.
"I consider it a gift. Everyone I have ever known considers it a gift," she
says.
She has a doctorate in English literature but earns her living as a
"reflectionist" photographing almost exclusively reflections on water. When the
light hits the water, she literally hears music.
"I shoot when I hear a chord of color," she says.
Scientific evidence
While such stories may seem hard to believe, modern science is beginning to
lend credibility to them.
At last month's Society for Neuroscience meeting in New Orleans, researchers
from the United States and Europe shared their recent findings on synesthesia.
In one study a team of scientists at Oxford University interviewed people who
said they had experienced colored hearing all their lives. They were asked to
describe what colors they saw when hearing the names of each day of the week,
month of the year, and letter of the alphabet and of several numbers between one
and 100. Two months later, they were surprised with another, identical pop quiz.
Their answers were, for the most part, the same.
"This is strong evidence that they were experiencing a genuine phenomenon.
They actually appeared to be seeing colors in their mind's eye," said Megan
Steven, the lead author of the study, in a prepared statement. Even more
impressive: the subjects had all been blind for at least 10 years.
In another study, which Ramachandran describes in the Scientific American
article, synesthetes were asked to look at a set of black 2s in the shape of a
triangle, hidden in a sea of black 5s. To most viewers, it would be hard to
distinguish the 2s from the 5s and the black-and-white sea. But for synesthetes,
for whom 2s and 5s are different colors, the triangle of 2s popped out
immediately.
In another experiment using brain imaging techniques, synesthetes presented
with a sequence of numbers showed activity in not only the portion of the brain
that processes numbers, but also the one that processes color.
And the researchers are just getting started, Grossenbacher says.
He has been in contact with more than 400 synesthetes across the nation and
hopes to interview as many as possible.
He says better understanding the trait may help prevent those who have it
from being misunderstood.
"I had one woman who, as a teen, was sent to drug rehab after a teacher
overheard her describing something she had seen," he said. Another was placed on
medication to treat what her doctor perceived as hallucinations.
Duffy is delighted to, at last, be connecting with others like her around the
country.
But she thinks her neurological quirk is just one example of how differently
human beings — synesthetic or not — can perceive the world around them.
"Anytime any of us looks at the world, we are seeing it as no one else has
seen it before. We are seeing it through a lens that no one else," she says.
"Maybe synesthesia just gives us a very concrete example of that."